Trajectory Analysis for Identifying Classes of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) in Children of the United States
RESEARCH ARTICLE

Trajectory Analysis for Identifying Classes of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) in Children of the United States

Clinical Practice & Epidemiology in Mental Health 21 May 2024 RESEARCH ARTICLE DOI: 10.2174/0117450179298863240516070510

Abstract

Background

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is a mental health disorder that affects attention and behavior. People with ADHD frequently encounter challenges in social interactions, facing issues, like social rejection and difficulties in interpersonal relationships, due to their inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity.

Methods

A National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY) database was employed to identify patterns of ADHD symptoms. The children who were born to women in the NLSY study between 1986 and 2014 were included. A total of 1,847 children in the NLSY 1979 cohort whose hyperactivity/inattention score was calculated when they were four years old were eligible for this study. A trajectory modeling method was used to evaluate the trajectory classes. Sex, baseline antisocial score, baseline anxiety score, and baseline depression score were adjusted to build the trajectory model. We used stepwise multivariate logistic regression models to select the risk factors for the identified trajectories.

Results

The trajectory analysis identified six classes for ADHD, including (1) no sign class, (2) few signs since preschool being persistent class, (3) few signs in preschool but no signs later class, (4) few signs in preschool that magnified in elementary school class, (5) few signs in preschool that diminished later class, and (6) many signs since preschool being persistent class. The sensitivity analysis resulted in a similar trajectory pattern, except for the few signs since preschool that magnified later class. Children’s race, breastfeeding status, headstrong score, immature dependent score, peer conflict score, educational level of the mother, baseline antisocial score, baseline anxious/depressed score, and smoking status 12 months prior to the birth of the child were found to be risk factors in the ADHD trajectory classes.

Conclusion

The trajectory classes findings obtained in the current study can (a) assist a researcher in evaluating an intervention (or combination of interventions) that best decreases the long-term impact of ADHD symptoms and (b) allow clinicians to better assess as to which class a child with ADHD belongs so that appropriate intervention can be employed.

Keywords: Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, Neurodevelopmental disorder, Preschool, Children, Families, Hyperactivity, Inattention, Impulsivity.

1. INTRODUCTION

People with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) frequently encounter challenges in social interactions, facing issues, like social rejection and difficulties in interpersonal relationships, due to their inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity [1-9]. These adverse social consequences lead to children’s emotional distress and anguish as well as parental stress levels [7]. The prevalence of ADHD in children aged 2 to 17 has been reported to vary from 2-13%, with specific rates for different age groups, described as follows: 2-5 years (2%), 6-11 years (10%), and 12-17 years (13%) [10-15]. Treatment and healthcare costs for ADHD can be an additional $15,000 annually compared to children without the disorder [16-18].

1.1. Assessment and Treatment of ADHD

The American Psychiatric Association (APA) classifies ADHD into three main types: predominantly inattentive presentation, predominantly hyperactive/impulsive presentation, or combined presentation [19]. Diagnosis relies on observations, assignments, and the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edition (DSM-5-TR) criteria [20]. Despite lacking biomarkers or conclusive neuroimaging differences, ADHD is recognized as a neurodevelopmental disorder in the DSM-5-TR [19]. Diagnosis involves initial screening, comprehensive assessment (evaluating symptoms' specifics, duration, and impact on functioning), and bio-psychosocial evaluation [21]. Multimodal treatments, including parent training, medication management, counseling, and educational support, are commonly used [22-27]. Tailoring interventions to individual needs improves effectiveness [1, 28], although prevention strategies can also mitigate ADHD risk [29].

1.2. Mental Health and Medical-related Issues

As one of the most common neurobehavioral disorders, ADHD is often associated with disruptive, mood, anxiety, and substance abuse [30-32]. Studies across various disciplines and healthcare sectors have demonstrated that the interplay between these health interactions can exacerbate symptoms, complicate treatment regimens, and diminish overall health outcomes [33-38]. While 5% of children have issues with overactivity, inattention, and impulsivity [39], an estimated 80% of children diagnosed with ADHD also have at least one other psychiatric disorder during their lifetime. Physical consequences, such as cardiovascular disease, are potential long-term consequences of continued pharmacological interventions used to treat ADHD [40-43]. Moreover, the DSM IV-TR did not allow for the dual diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) and ADHD, whereas the DSM 5 and DSM 5-TR have allowed for the inclusion of these co-occurring diagnoses [44]. In fact, some studies estimate that the comorbidity rate of ADHD with learning disorders (17%) and ASD (49%) may be due to an overlap in symptoms [45, 46].

1.3. Machine Learning and ADHD

Machine learning analyses have been conducted on pre-existing data for children with ADHD in an attempt to determine better strategies for assessing the severity of symptoms [47]. These studies have investigated the use of Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) [47] and Positron Emission Tomography (PET) imaging in machine learning to identify clusters of symptoms [48], but this process has yet to become clinically meaningful [49]. The use of machine learning classifiers for differentiating between multiple psychiatric conditions is based on clinical records among the ADHD population, and there is some benefit for healthcare professionals in categorizing symptoms across different domains, rather than specific domains [50].

Analyzing trajectories in ADHD research is complex and offers valuable insights into predictors, the developmental path over time, variations in symptoms, prognosis, and treatment responses. Research has shown children with ADHD who do not receive treatment to have poorer long-term outcomes in 9 major categories (i.e., academic, antisocial behavior, driving, non-medicinal drug use/addictive behavior, obesity, occupation, service use, self-esteem, and social function) compared to those who have received treatment, although outcomes have not been reported to improve to normal levels [38]. Extending this prior research to further cluster symptoms within categories of severity (and observe over a length of time) can (1) aid healthcare professionals in educating families on prevention, (2) enable researchers to design tailored interventions based on specific classes instead of a generic approach, and (3) guide the selection of evidence-based treatments tailored to individual classes once interventions are developed and studied. Hence, this study aimed to utilize the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY) database to identify patterns of ADHD symptoms. This identification will help inform diverse prevention strategies in clinical and public health settings. Specifically, we aimed to explore the existence of distinct classes of ADHD symptoms and the variables associated with these classes. While prior research has created clusters of symptom groupings for ADHD, we are exploring how hyperactivity/inattention symptoms change (or do not change) over a length of time.

2. MATERIALS AND METHODS

2.1. Source of Data

The NLSY is a set of surveys that collect multiple information points on the labor market activities and other significant life events of various groups of men and women. The first cohort (NLSY79), sponsored by the United States (US) Bureau of Labor Statistics, included 12,686 persons aged 14 to 22, with the survey beginning in 1979. In this present study, we used the NLSY79 Child and Young Adult cohort (NLSY79CYA), funded by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, which follows the children who were born to the NLSY79 women cohort between 1986 and 2018. To date, 11,545 children have been followed up and interviewed up to 17 rounds in that period (National Longitudinal Surveys, https://nlsinfo.org/content/cohorts/ nlsy79-children). The NLSY and its associated databases have been proven essential in various research fields, including public health [51]. In addition, this longitudinal survey has high retention rates due to the careful design, making it suitable for life course research [51, 52].

2.2. Participants

The NLSY79CYA is a public database that does not contain any personal identifiers. After approval by the institutional review board of the primary author’s university (University of Illinois at Springfield; IRB approval number 24-003), 1,847 children born between 1986 and 2014 in the NLSY79CYA cohort whose hyperactivity/inattention score was calculated when they started this study at the age of four were included in the analysis. Two children born in 2014 were the last cohort included in the analysis. Their hyperactivity/inattention scores were recorded in 2018 when they were four years old.

2.3. ADHD and Other Predictive Variables

The NLSY79CYA collected information on children’s sex, race, prenatal care (e.g., mother had taken vitamins during pregnancy, mother drank alcohol/smoked during 12 months before the birth of the child), whether premature birth or not, low birth weight (5.5 pounds or less), and postnatal care (e.g., breastfeed). The Behavior Problems Index (BPI), developed by Nicholas Zill and James Peterson, was used to evaluate children's behavioral problems. It measures the frequency, range, and types of behavior problems in children aged four and above [53]. The BPI consists of items taken from the Achenbach Behavior Problems Checklist [54] and other child behavior scales [55-57]. The BPI is a tool used to evaluate children's behavior based on six domains: anxious/depressed, headstrong, hyperactive, immature dependency, anti-social, and peer conflict/social withdrawal. Mothers report measurements using 3-level items [58]. The hyperactivity score was first obtained when a child was four years old. The scores were then repeatedly measured in the subsequent five rounds of surveys at ages 6, 8, 10, 12, and 14. Based on the BPI, the NLSY79CYA defines the strength of the hyperactivity/inattention score (ADHD signs) using the following five signs (0-5 points): The child 1) has difficulty concentrating/paying attention; 2) is easily confused and seems in a fog; 3) is impulsive or acts without thinking; 4) has trouble getting his/her mind off certain thoughts; 5) is restless, overly active, and cannot sit still.

A child is defined by the antisocial score (0-6 points) as 1) cheats or tells lies; 2) bullies or is cruel/mean to others; 3) does not seem to feel sorry after misbehaving; 4) breaks things deliberately; 5) is disobedient at school; and 6) has trouble getting along with teachers. Anxious/depressed (0-5 points) is defined as follows: 1) has sudden changes in mood or feeling; 2) feels/complains no one loves him/her; 3) is too fearful or anxious; 4) feels worthless or inferior; and 5) is unhappy, sad, or depressed. A headstrong child (0-5 points) refers to he/she 1) is rather high-strung, tense, and nervous; 2) argues too much; 3) is disobedient at home; 4) is stubborn, sullen, or irritable; and 5) has a strong temper and loses it easily. Dependent (0-4 points) means that the child 1) clings to adults; 2) cries too much; 3) demands a lot of attention; and 4) is too dependent on others. Peer problems score (0-3 points) was measured by asking if the child 1) has trouble getting along with other children; 2) is not liked by other children; and 3) is withdrawn and does not get involved with others. The above BPI scores were measured along with the ADHD signs at 4, 6, 8, 10, 12, and 14 years of age.

2.4. Statistical Analyses

A trajectory modeling method, a latent class modeling approach, was used to evaluate one or more outcomes over age or time, as in a repeat measurement from a longitudinal study design [59, 60]. One of the members of the trajectory modeling technique family is Group-based Trajectory Modelling (GBTM) [60-62], which is a semi-parametric finite mixture model designed to identify clusters of individuals following a similar progression of some behavior over time [60, 63, 64]. The GBTM assumes that the population comprises a discrete number of distinct groups that can distinguish subgroups/classes of homogeneous individuals by their behavior profiles [63, 64]. It can help identify the uncertainty of latent group membership based on multiple risk factors that influence group membership decision-making [65-69].

2.5. Trajectory Model Building

The first step of building the GBTM is determining the number of and the polynomial order of trajectory classes in a population that best fits the data [63, 70]. A SAS PROC TRAJ procedure was used to build the models. We started from a one-class model with a quartic degree polynomial because the PROC TRAJ procedure does not allow a polynomial order greater than four (quartic). We then increased the number of classes with quartic degree polynomials until the model fit the data according to the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) and Bayes factor. When the number of classes was confirmed, the second step was to decrease the polynomial orders of the classes until the highest order polynomial for each class was statistically significant (p < 0.05). We used the logged Bayes factor approximation [2*(BICj-BICi)] proposed by Jeffreys and Kass and Raftery to determine the best-fit model [71]. When the Bayes Factor value exceeds 10, it indicates a strong preference for model j over model i. The value between 6 and 10 suggests a moderate preference for model j over model i. If the value is between 2 and 6, it shows some evidence that model j is favored over model i. However, when the value is less than 2, it suggests no difference between the models; thus, model i should be chosen.

Although ADHD affects both boys and girls, previous studies suggest the disease to be more prevalent in boys than girls [72, 73]. Studies have found ADHD to be associated with antisocial behavior [74, 75]. ADHD and depression can coexist. About one in two adults with ADHD and one in three children with ADHD also have an anxiety disorder [76-80]. Thus, sex, baseline antisocial score, baseline anxiety score, and baseline depression score were adjusted as we built the trajectory model. We also conducted the sensitivity analysis, which only included children who had fully completed the six-round surveys with ten years of follow-up.

2.6. Stepwise Logistic Regression

We used stepwise multivariate logistic regression models to select the risk factors for the identified trajectories. A SAS PROC LOGISTIC procedure was performed to determine the most parsimonious model using a significant level of 0.20 set for entry and 0.05 for stay [63, 70, 81, 82]. All analyses were performed using SAS package version 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc, NC).

3. RESULTS

Six rounds of surveys were available for the analysis. The baseline/first-round survey comprised 1,847 four-year-old children who enrolled in the NLYS79CYA cohort between 1986 and 2016. The subsequent five rounds repeatedly measured children at 6-, 8-, 10-, 12-, and 14 years of age involving 1,607, 1,515, 1,470, 1,406, and 1,355 children, respectively (Table 1), accounting for a total of 1,000 children who fully completed the six round surveys with ten years of follow-up.

The first-round survey at four years old involved 917 (49.7%) boys and 930 (50.3%) girls; 52.7% were White, followed by Black children (25.6%) and Hispanic (21.7%). In general, there were more girls (50.3% to 51.6%) than boys (48.4% to 49.2%) in the follow-up rounds. Children who were White constituted the majority of the surveys (51.3% to 52.7%). Furthermore, little more than 50% of the children were breastfed, 21.8% had premature births, and 7.3% were low birth weight babies. The children’s mothers reported that an average of 44.7% drank alcohol and 28.0% smoked during the 12 months before the birth of their child. The mean and Standard Deviation (SD) of baseline antisocial, anxious/depressed, headstrong, dependent, and peer conflicts/withdrawn scores are presented in Table 1.

3.1. Trajectory Classes of ADHD Signs

The trajectory analysis identified six classes (Table 2). We included race, mother’s education level, premature birth, low birth weight, breastfeeding, mother had taken vitamins during pregnancy, mother drank alcohol/smoked during 12 months before the birth of the child, headstrong score, immature dependency score, and peer conflict/social withdrawal score in the stepwise multivariate logistic regression models. The final model indicated race, mother’s education level, mother smoked during 12 months before the birth of the child, breastfeeding, headstrong score, dependent score, and peer conflict/social withdrawal score as associated with the trajectory classes (Tables S1-S6). The following key comparisons among the six classes were based on the findings of the descriptive statistics in Table 3 and the multivariate logistic regressions in Tables S1-S6.

Table 1.
Sample characteristics and ADHD predictors by age.
- 4 Years 6 Years 8 Years 10 Years 12 Years 14 Years
Number of children in the survey 1847 1607 1515 1470 1406 1355
Hyperactivity/inattention score (0-5; mean, SD) 1.79 (1.46) 1.70 (1.53) 1.72 (1.57) 1.65 (1.60) 1.53 (1.55) 1.46 (1.54)
Sex - - - - - -
Boy 917 (49.7%) 781 (48.6%) 733 (48.4%) 715 (48.6%) 682 (48.5%) 667 (49.2%)
Girl 930 (50.3%) 826 (51.4%) 782 (51.6%) 755 (51.4%) 724 (51.5%) 688 (50.3%)
Race - - - - - -
Hispanic 401 (21.7%) 354 (22.0%) 328 (21.7%) 322 (21.9%) 294 (20.9%) 287 (21.3%)
Black 472 (25.6%) 422 (26.3%) 398 (26.3%) 392 (26.7%) 379 (27.0%) 317 (27.4%)
White, non-Hispanic 974 (52.7%) 831 (51.7%) 789 (52.0%) 756 (51.4%) 733 (52.1%) 695 (51.3%)
Premature birth - - - - - -
No 1445 (78.2%) - - - - -
Yes 402 (21.8%) - - - - -
Low birth weight - - - - - -
No 1537 (92.7%) - - - - -
Yes 121 (7.3%) - - - - -
Breastfeed - - - - - -
No 858 (48.7%) - - - - -
Yes 902 (51.3%) - - - - -
Mother’s education level (year, SD) 12.8 (2.46) - - - - -
Mother drank alcohol during 12 months before the birth of child (yes) - - - - - -
No 931 (55.3%) - - - - -
Yes 752 (44.7%) - - - - -
Mother smoked during 12 months before the birth of the child (yes) - - - - - -
No 1203 (72.0%) - - - - -
Yes 467 (28.0%) - - - - -
Antisocial score (0-6; mean, SD) 1.20 (1.20) - - - - -
Anxious/depressed score (0-5; mean, SD) 1.16 (1.17) - - - - -
Headstrong score (0-5; mean, SD) 2.36 (1.61) - - - - -
Dependent score (0-4; mean, SD) 1.71 (1.29) - - - - -
Peer conflicts/withdrawn score (0-3; mean, SD) 0.40 (0.71) - - - - -
Table 2.
Model fit for 1-7 class quartic group-based trajectory analysis.
Model Number of Class BIC Jeffreys and Kass and Raftery Approximation
2*(BICj-BICi)
Model Comparison
(j to i)
Evidence for or Against
1 One -16293.82 -- -- --
2 Two -14821.21 2945.22 Model 2 to model 1 Very strong evidence against model 1
3 Three -14532.50 577.42 Model 3 to model 2 Very strong evidence against model 2
4 Four -14442.39 180.22 Model 4 to model 3 Very strong evidence against model 3
5 Five -14391.91 100.96 Model 5 to model 4 Very strong evidence against model 4
6 Six -14373.36 37.10 Model 6 to model 5 Strong evidence against model 5
7 Seven -14390.67 -34.62 Model 7 to model 6 No evidence against model 6
Table 3.
Descriptive statistics for six classes of ADHD.
- C1
(No Sign)
C2
(Few Signs Since Preschool Being Persistent)
C3
(Few Signs in Preschool but No Sign Later)
C4
(Few Signs in Preschool that Magnified in Elementary School)
C5
(Few Signs in Preschool that Diminished Later)
C6
(Many Signs Since Preschool being Persistent)
% of children 8.4% (n=160) 22.7% (n=418) 11.8% (n=200) 27.4% (n=544) 20.4% (n=341) 9.3% (n=162)
Sex - - - - - -
Boy 58 (36.3%) 168 (40.2%) 74 (37.0%) 338 (62.1%) 158 (46.3%) 111 (68.5%)
Girl 102 (63.7%) 250 (59.8%) 126 (63.0%) 206 (37.9%) 183 (53.7%) 51 (31.5%)
Race - - - - - -
Hispanic 33 (20.6%) 82 (19.6%) 49 (24.5%) 128 (23.5%) 72 (21.1%) 31 (19.1%)
Black 28 (17.5%) 104 (24.9%) 42 (21.0%) 129 (23.7%) 113 (33.1%) 52 (32.1%)
White, non-Hispanic 99 (61.9%) 232 (55.5%) 109 (54.5%) 287 (52.8%) 156 (45.8%) 79 (48.8%)
Premature birth - - - - - -
No 130 (81.3%) 330 (79.0%) 149 (74.5%) 432 (79.4%) 257 (75.4%) 128 (79.0%)
Yes 30 (18.7%) 88 (21.0%) 51 (25.5%) 112 (20.6%) 84 (24.6%) 34 (21.0%)
Low birth weight - - - - - -
No 136 (94.4%) 348 (93.3%) 164 (92.7%) 452 (92.6%) 278 (90.9%) 140 (93.3%)
Yes 8 (5.6%) 25 (6.7%) 13 (7.3%) 36 (7.4%) 28 (9.1%) 10 (6.7%)
Breastfeed - - - - - -
No 49 (31.4%) 182 (46.0%) 91 (47.2%) 271 (52.2%) 164 (51.1%) 91 (58.3%)
Yes 107 (68.6%) 214 (54.0%) 102 (52.8%) 248 (47.8%) 157 (48.9%) 65 (41.7%)
Mother’s education level (year, SD) 13.6 (2.40) 13.3 (2.46) 12.9 (2.52) 12.4 (2.46) 12.6 (2.31) 11.9 (2.16)
Mother took vitamins during pregnancy - - - - - -
No 3 (2.0%) 16 (4.2%) 8 (4.6%) 27 (5.5%) 12 (4.0%) 5 (3.3%)
Yes 144 (98.0%) 361 (95.8%) 166 (95.4) 468 (94.5%) 292 (96.0%) 147 (96.7%)
Mother drank alcohol during 12 months before the birth of child (yes) - - - - - -
No 83 (56.5%) 194 (51.1%) 102 (57.6%) 274 (54.9%) 177 (57.5%) 87 (57.2%)
Yes 64 (43.5%) 186 (48.9%) 75 (42.4%) 225 (45.1%) 131 (42.5%) 65 (42.8%)
Mother smoked during 12 months before the birth of the child (yes) - - - - - -
No 120 (82.2%) 291 (76.8%) 134 (76.1%) 333 (67.3%) 222 (73.3%) 86 (57.0%)
Yes 26 (17.8%) 88 (23.2%) 42 (23.9%) 162 (32.7%) 81 (26.7%) 65 (43.0%)
Baseline antisocial score (0-6; mean, SD) 0.20 (0.45) 0.54 (0.75) 0.91 (1.00) 1.19 (1.03) 1.92 (1.13) 2.79 (1.05)
Baseline anxious/depressed score (0-5; mean, SD) 0.25 (0.50) 0.59 (0.73) 0.91 (0.88) 0.99 (0.87) 2.12 (1.22) 2.46 (1.31)
Baseline headstrong score (0-5; mean, SD) 0.72 (1.05) 1.55 (1.34) 1.84 (1.43) 2.60 (1.45) 3.19 (1.31) 4.03 (1.11)
Baseline dependent score (0-4; mean, SD) 0.75 (1.10) 1.28 (1.16) 1.54 (1.21) 1.78 (1.21) 2.17 (1.18) 2.74 (1.25)
Baseline peer conflicts/withdrawn score (0-3; mean, SD) 0.06 (0.24) 0.15 (0.41) 0.29 (0.59) 0.36 (0.62) 0.67 (0.86) 1.03 (0.99)

3.2. Class C1

C1 class (no sign) involved the most girls with the lowest baseline antisocial, anxious/depressed, headstrong, dependent, and peer conflict scores compared to other classes (Table 3). This class of children had the highest proportion of breastfeeding (68.6%) (Tables 3 and S1). Compared to other classes, their mothers had the highest education levels (13.6 years), the highest vitamin use during pregnancy (98.0%), and the lowest smoking rate 12 months before the birth of the child (17.8%). Children in C1 (8.4%) showed no ADHD signs during the 10-year follow-up.

3.3. Classes C2 and C3

C2 (few signs since preschool being persistent) and C3 (few signs in preschool but no sign later) classes were similar. C2 children (22.7%) were found to have few signs at their preschool age; the signs persisted throughout the follow-up rounds until the age of 14. These children were considered to be few signs since preschool being persistent class. C3 class (11.8%) included a group of children with few signs at preschool age (Fig. 1). In contrast to the C2 class, their ADHD signs diminished after they were 12 years old. They were classified as few signs in preschool but no signs later class. Children in both classes had few signs/symptoms since preschool age. However, C3 children had no signs as they grew up, while the signs in the C2 class persisted throughout the surveys. Both classes primarily involved girls (59.8% and 63.0%, respectively) (Table 3). Compared to the C1 class, children in the C2 and C3 classes had higher BPI scores (Table S1). C2 mothers had the highest proportion of alcohol drinking during the 12 months before the birth of the child (48.9%), compared to other classes. In addition, C3 children had the most premature birth (25.5%) compared to other classes. Fig. 2 (A-E) also demonstrated that among the C3 children, the antisocial, immature dependency, and peer conflict scores decreased in the follow-up rounds. On the other hand, the anxious/depressed score increased in C2 children as they grew up (Fig. 2B).

Fig. (1).

ADHD symptom trajectories for the 6-class model.

3.4. Classes C4 and C5

Children in the C4 class (Few signs in preschool that magnified in elementary school) and C5 class (few signs in preschool that diminished later) showed a few signs at baseline (C4 children = 27.4%; C5 children = 20.4%); their (C5) ADHD signs continued diminishing significantly as they grew up to the age of 14. However, for C4 children, the signs were magnified in elementary school (Fig. 1). The C4 class involved more White boys than C5 (Tables 3 and S4). C5 class had more participants within the Black race than C1–C4 (Table S5). Although the C5 children had higher BPI scores at age four than the C4 children, the scores tended to decrease afterward. Eventually, the BPI scores in the C5 class were lower than that of the C4 class (Fig. 2).

3.5. Class C6

Children in the C6 class (9.3%) experienced the most signs reported by their mothers. In addition, those signs were persistent throughout the follow-up (Fig. 1). They were likelier to be Black boys whose mothers had lower education levels (11.9 years) than all other classes (Table S6). Also, their mothers had the highest proportion of smoking during the pregnancy (42.5%). Children in this class had the highest BPI scores throughout the follow-up period (Fig. 2, Tables 3, and S6).

Fig. (2).

BPI scores at ages 4, 6, 8, 10, 12, and 14 in 6 trajectories.

Fig. (3).

Sensitivity analysis result of ADHD symptom trajectories for the 6-class model.

3.6. Sensitivity Analysis

We included 1,000 out of 1,847 children who fully completed the six-round surveys with ten years of follow-up in the sensitivity analysis. The trajectory analysis also suggested six classes for the ADHD sign patterns (Fig. 3 and Table S7). The trajectory pattern of C1 (5.3%), C3 (13.9%), C4 (31.4%), C5 (20.1%), and C6 (9.0%) classes was similar to the whole population. C2 children (20.4%) were found to have few signs at their preschool age; the signs were magnified after they entered elementary school and continued to magnify until age 14. The pattern of this class was slightly different from that of the whole population. These children were defined as a class with a few signs since preschool that magnified later.

4. DISCUSSION

ADHD is highly heterogeneous, which has made it extremely challenging for researchers to identify the underlying pathophysiology, developmental trajectories, and effective interventions at the individual level. This study used the NLSY79CYA longitudinal cohort to develop the ADHD trajectories among 1,847 children who were followed up to six rounds between 1986 and 2018. The hyperactivity/inattention scores of two children born in 2014 were obtained in 2018 when they were four years old. Although only one data point was provided in the analyses, the influence on the trajectory patterns could be ignored. In the sensitivity analysis including only children who fully completed the six-round surveys, we did not find significantly different patterns from the whole population. We found the optimal ADHD sign trajectory classes to be no sign class (C1), few signs since preschool being persistent class (C2), few signs in preschool but no signs later class (C3), few signs in preschool that magnified in elementary school class (C4), few signs in preschool that diminished later class (C5), and many signs since preschool being persistent class (C6). The sensitivity analysis resulted in a similar trajectory pattern, except for the few signs since preschool that magnified later class.

The patterns of no sign class (C1) and few signs since preschool being persistent class (C2) were consistent with a previous UK study. However, in our study, 8.4% were in the no sign class, compared to 34.9%, and 22.7% of children were in the few signs since preschool being persistent class compared to 24.1% in the UK population [83]. In another study [84], the authors called the C1 class as “persisting low,” which comprised 38.2% of their study population. The few signs since preschool being persistent class (C2) showed slightly elevated signs compared to the whole population. However, in the sensitivity analysis, we found that this class of children showed significantly magnified signs throughout the follow-up. Compared to children with completed hyperactivity/inattention score records, children who missed at least one round of the survey had lower hyperactivity/inattention scores at ages 10, 12, and 14. These children also had higher antisocial and headstrong scores than the fully surveyed children. Their mothers’ education level was slightly lower than that of the fully surveyed children (13.1 years vs. 13.5 years). In the sensitivity analysis, children in the few signs since preschool that magnified later class (C2) had higher baseline peer conflict scores than the whole population.

The findings of a few signs in preschool but no sign later class (C3) were in accordance with Leopold and colleagues’ study findings. Their study found that hyperactivity/impulsivity declined from preschool through ninth grade (14 years old) in a community twin sample [85]. Another study among children from low-income families followed from kindergarten through third grade (8 years old) also demonstrated a similar pattern [86]. Those children had few signs of inattention in kindergarten but they declined later. Many children with higher levels of ADHD signs [e.g., a few signs in preschool that magnified in elementary school class (C4) and many signs since preschool being persistent class (C6)] developed ADHD during the school-age years [87]. The findings in this present study of a few signs in preschool that magnified in elementary school class (C4) and many signs since preschool being persistent class (C6) have been found to be consistent with previous studies. A few signs in preschool that magnified in elementary school class (C4) was also called “persisters” in O’Neill’s study. They described that children in this class had high ADHD symptoms level in preschool age and it continued to persist with higher level symptoms throughout the school age. They eventually met the diagnostic criteria for ADHD [87]. The many signs since preschool being persistent class (C6) was the classical ADHD trajectory class found in previous studies [83, 88]. This class of children included less girls and had more behavior/conduct problems than other classes. This finding has been found to be consistent with other studies [83, 89]. A previous study also called it “pre-school onset being persistent.” [83] A study was conducted on the US children recruited from schools located in high-risk communities across four states (NC, TN, WA, and PA) between 3rd and 12th grades. The study found a similar class, but it did not include younger children, unlike the current study [90].

The findings of a few signs in preschool that diminished later class (C5) have also been found to be consistent with previous studies [83, 84, 87]. In O’Neill’s study, this class was defined as “preschool-limited”. Murray and colleagues called it “subclinical remitting.” Tandon and colleagues called it “gradually remitting.” A study investigating the trajectory patterns among US children between 3 and 5 years of age also found that some children demonstrated a similar pattern. Those children were reported with high ADHD symptoms at age 3. Some ADHD symptoms diminished as they grew up to 5 years old [88].

4.1. Risk Factors

Understanding the factors that influence trajectories in various phenomena is crucial for informed decision-making and effective interventions. However, identifying the risk factors associated with these trajectories is pivotal, as it not only enhances our comprehension of the underlying processes, but also enables the development of targeted and timely interventions. By uncovering the elements that predispose individuals to specific trajectories, researchers and policymakers gain valuable insights that can inform preventive strategies [91], personalized treatments, and public health initiatives. The current study’s findings showed children’s race, breastfeeding status, headstrong score, immature dependent score, peer conflict score, educational level of the mother, baseline antisocial score, baseline anxious/depressed score, and smoking status 12 months prior to the birth of the child as risk factors in the ADHD trajectory classes (after adjusting for children’s sex, baseline antisocial score, and baseline anxious/depressed score).

4.2. Race and Educational Level

Race was a potential risk factor among the trajectory classes. Studies have found the minority children (e.g., African Americans or Hispanics) to be much less likely than identical White children to receive an ADHD diagnosis [92, 93]. These racial differences in ADHD diagnosis occur as early as kindergarten and continue until middle school. These children are also less likely to be using medication to treat the disorder by the end of elementary and middle school [92]. Studies have also revealed maternal breastfeeding to be an important factor for a lower risk of ADHD in children [94, 95]. This has been found to be consistent with our findings in this study.

Mother’s education level has also been found to be a predictor for the trajectory classes [83]. Studies have found children with low ADHD symptom classes to have mothers with higher education levels compared to those with high ADHD symptom classes. Prenatal smoking has been found to be a risk factor for ADHD trajectory classes. High ADHD symptoms have been found to be associated with a higher proportion of mothers’ prenatal smoking [88, 96, 97].

The limitations of the current study are as follows: secondary data sources can have limitations in terms of accuracy, completeness, and relevance. The quality of the trajectory analysis heavily depends on the quality of the data source, and inaccurate or incomplete data could lead to biased results or limited generalizability of findings. Additionally, specific variables (e.g., ADHD diagnosis – yes/no) that could generate greater discovery may limit the depth of the analysis and the ability to draw meaningful conclusions. Secondary data may have also lacked detailed contextual information related to the individuals in the dataset, which more rigorous non-quasi-experimental analysis or qualitative methodologies may provide greater context as to why the trends we found have existed. Specific to the current study, the hyperactive index only investigated part of the ADHD symptoms. A comprehensive screening and diagnosis by a licensed clinician based on the DSM-5-TR, coupled with the variables identified within the current study, would yield more robust predictions that could result in earlier intervention and determine the best course of intervention. Finally, we have not included the family history of prior ADHD diagnosis in our predictive model (data were unavailable). It is strongly supported that parents’ diagnosis of ADHD strongly predicts whether the child will have a similar diagnosis [98].

CONCLUSION

Previous studies have demonstrated two key findings: firstly, environmental influences play a significant role in shaping both the structure and function of the developing brain, and secondly, alterations in brain structure and function are directly linked to ADHD as individuals progress through developmental stages [29]. Notably, research has emphasized the critical window of opportunity during the preschool years when the brain exhibits higher plasticity, making it more receptive to lasting modifications, and prior to the emergence of complicating factors that can hinder treatment efficacy.

Interventions implemented during this early stage have been proven to mitigate the long-term impact of ADHD trajectories. Recent studies have underscored the positive outcomes of preschool-age interventions, revealing significant improvements in social skills and reductions in behavioral problems according to assessments from teachers and parents [99, 100]. Additionally, research has highlighted the effectiveness of early intervention programs tailored for young children aged 2 to 5, emphasizing the enhancement of self-regulatory behaviors and fostering stronger interpersonal relationships between educators and parents [101, 102]. Among the consistently successful strategies found in these intervention programs have been parent management techniques and preschool teacher training. These methods encompass diverse learning materials, such as parent-child interaction training, psychoeducation, behavioral interventions within the preschool setting, and child-focused approaches, like social and positive reinforcement for adhering to rules [103].

Using the trajectory classes as identified in the current study can (a) assist a researcher in evaluating an intervention (or combination of interventions) that best decreases the long-term impact of ADHD symptoms and (b) allow clinicians to better assess which class a child with ADHD belongs to so that appropriate intervention can be employed.

LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS

NLSY79 = 1979 Cohort of National Longitudinal Survey of Youth
NLSY79CYA = NLSY79 Child and Young Adult
ADHD = Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder
DSM-5-TR = Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edition
BIC = Bayesian Information Criterion
BPI = Behavior Problems Index
GBTM = Group-based Trajectory Model

ETHICS APPROVAL AND CONSENT TO PARTICIPATE

The institutional review board of the primary author’s university (University of Illinois at Springfield) approved the study with number IRB 24-003.

HUMAN AND ANIMAL RIGHTS

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the standards of institutional and/or research committee, and with the 1975 Declaration of Helsinki, as revised in 2013.

CONSENT FOR PUBLICATION

Not applicable.

STANDARDS OF REPORTING

STROBE guidelines were followed.

AVAILABILITY OF DATA AND MATERIALS

The anonymized NLSY data collected are available as open database at https://nlsinfo.org/content/cohorts/ nlsy79-children.

FUNDING

None.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

The authors declare no conflict of interest, financial or otherwise.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

The authors thank the National Longitudinal Surveys, sponsored by the US Bureau of Labor Statistics.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL

Supplementary material is available on the publisher’s website along with the published article.

REFERENCES

1
CHADD. Treatment overview. 2023. Available From: https://chadd.org/for-parents/treatment-overview/
2
Barkley RA. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment New York: Guilford. Fox, DJ, Tharp, DF, & Fox, LC (2005). Neurofeedback: An alternative and efficacious treatment for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback 1998; 30: 365-73.
3
Flicek M. Social status of boys with both academic problems and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1992; 20(4): 353-66.
4
Sodano SM, Tamulonis JP, Fabiano GA, et al. Interpersonal problems of young adults with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Atten Disord 2021; 25(4): 562-71.
5
Carpenter Rich E, Loo SK, Yang M, Dang J, Smalley SL. Social functioning difficulties in ADHD: Association with PDD risk. Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry 2009; 14(3): 329-44.
6
Nijmeijer JS, Minderaa RB, Buitelaar JK, Mulligan A, Hartman CA, Hoekstra PJ. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and social dysfunctioning. Clin Psychol Rev 2008; 28(4): 692-708.
7
Gagliano A, Lamberti M, Siracusano R, et al. A comparison between children with ADHD and children with epilepsy in self-esteem and parental stress level. Clin Pract Epidemiol Ment Health 2014; 10(1): 176-83.
8
Aduen PA, Day TN, Kofler MJ, Harmon SL, Wells EL, Sarver DE. Social problems in ADHD: Is it a skills acquisition or performance problem? J Psychopathol Behav Assess 2018; 40(3): 440-51.
9
Mikami AY, Smit S, Khalis A. Social skills training and ADHD—what works? Curr Psychiatry Rep 2017; 19(12): 93.
10
Danielson ML, Bitsko RH, Ghandour RM, Holbrook JR, Kogan MD, Blumberg SJ. Prevalence of parent-reported ADHD diagnosis and associated treatment among US children and adolescents, 2016. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 2018; 47(2): 199-212.
11
Rowland AS, Skipper BJ, Umbach DM, et al. The prevalence of ADHD in a population-based sample. J Atten Disord 2015; 19(9): 741-54.
12
Thomas R, Sanders S, Doust J, Beller E, Glasziou P. Prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatrics 2015; 135(4): e994-e1001.
13
Wolraich ML, McKeown RE, Visser SN, et al. The prevalence of ADHD. J Atten Disord 2014; 18(7): 563-75.
14
Bitsko RH, Claussen AH, Lichstein J, et al. Mental health surveillance among children—United States, 2013–2019. MMWR Suppl 2022; 71(2): 1-42.
15
Reuben CE. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children ages 5-17 years: United States, 2020-2022. NCHS Data Brief 2024; 2024(4992): 1-9.
16
Pelham WE, Foster EM, Robb JA. The economic impact of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. J Pediatr Psychol 2007; 32(6): 711-27.
17
Matza LS, Paramore C, Prasad M. A review of the economic burden of ADHD. Cost Eff Resour Alloc 2005; 3(1): 5.
18
Pan PY, Bölte S. The association between ADHD and physical health: A co-twin control study. Sci Rep 2020; 10(1): 22388.
19
Garayalde RES. What is ADHD? 2022. Available From: https://www.psychiatry.org/patients-families/adhd/what-is-adhd
20
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders 5th ed.. 2022.
21
Faraone SV, Larsson H. Genetics of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Mol Psychiatry 2019; 24(4): 562-75.
22
Hinshaw SP, Arnold LE, Group MC. Attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder, multimodal treatment, and longitudinal outcome: Evidence, paradox, and challenge. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci 2015; 6(1): 39-52.
23
Molina BSG, Hinshaw SP, Swanson JM, et al. The MTA at 8 years: Prospective follow-up of children treated for combined-type ADHD in a multisite study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2009; 48(5): 484-500.
24
CDC. Parent Training in Behavior Management for ADHD. 2022. Available From: https://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/adhd/behavior-therapy.html
25
Wolraich ML, Hagan JF Jr, Allan C, et al. Clinical practice guideline for the diagnosis, evaluation, and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Pediatrics 2019; 144(4): e20192528.
26
Harrison JR, Bunford N, Evans SW, Owens JS. Educational accommodations for students with behavioral challenges: A systematic review of the literature. Rev Educ Res 2013; 83(4): 551-97.
27
Moore DA, Russell AE, Matthews J, et al. School‐based interventions for attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review with multiple synthesis methods. Rev Educ 2018; 6(3): 209-63.
28
Webster-Stratton C, Reid J. Tailoring the Incredible Years™: Parent, teacher, and child interventions for young children with ADHD. 2014. Available From: https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2013-39115-006
29
Halperin JM, Bédard ACV, Curchack-Lichtin JT. Preventive interventions for ADHD: A neurodevelopmental perspective. Neurotherapeutics 2012; 9(3): 531-41.
30
Wilens TE, Spencer TJ. Understanding attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder from childhood to adulthood. Postgrad Med 2010; 122(5): 97-109.
31
Greenhill LL, Pliszka S, Dulcan MK, et al. Practice parameter for the use of stimulant medications in the treatment of children, adolescents, and adults. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2002; 41(2)(Suppl.): 26S-49S.
32
American Academy of Pediatrics. American Academy of Pediatrics Subcommittee on Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and Committee on Quality Improvement: Clinical practice guideline: treatment of the school-aged child with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics 2001; 108(4): 1033-44.
33
Nigg JT. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and adverse health outcomes. Clin Psychol Rev 2013; 33(2): 215-28.
34
Landes SD, London AS. Self-reported ADHD and adult health in the United States. J Atten Disord 2021; 25(1): 3-13.
35
Leppert B, Riglin L, Wootton RE, et al. The effect of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder on physical health outcomes: A 2-sample Mendelian randomization study. Am J Epidemiol 2021; 190(6): 1047-55.
36
Taipale H, Bergström J, Gèmes K, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder medications and work disability and mental health outcomes. JAMA Netw Open 2024; 7(3): e242859-9.
37
Schoenfelder EN, Kollins SH. Topical review: ADHD and health-risk behaviors: Toward prevention and health promotion. J Pediatr Psychol 2016; 41(7): 735-40.
38
Shaw M, Hodgkins P, Caci H, et al. A systematic review and analysis of long-term outcomes in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Effects of treatment and non-treatment. BMC Med 2012; 10(1): 99.
39
Sayal K, Prasad V, Daley D, Ford T, Coghill D. ADHD in children and young people: Prevalence, care pathways, and service provision. Lancet Psychiatry 2018; 5(2): 175-86.
40
Zhang L, Li L, Andell P, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder medications and long-term risk of cardiovascular diseases. JAMA Psychiatry 2024; 81(2): 178-87.
41
Mechler K, Banaschewski T, Hohmann S, Häge A. Evidence-based pharmacological treatment options for ADHD in children and adolescents. Pharmacol Ther 2022; 230: 107940.
42
Krinzinger H, Hall CL, Groom MJ, et al. Neurological and psychiatric adverse effects of long-term methylphenidate treatment in ADHD: A map of the current evidence. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2019; 107: 945-68.
43
Elliott J, Johnston A, Husereau D, et al. Pharmacologic treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults: A systematic review and network meta-analysis. PLoS One 2020; 15(10): e0240584.
44
Gnanavel S, Sharma P, Kaushal P, Hussain S. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and comorbidity: A review of literature. World J Clin Cases 2019; 7(17): 2420-6.
45
Stevens MC, Gaynor A, Bessette KL, Pearlson GD. A preliminary study of the effects of working memory training on brain function. Brain Imaging Behav 2016; 10(2): 387-407.
46
Mayes SD, Calhoun SL, Mayes RD, Molitoris S. Autism and ADHD: Overlapping and discriminating symptoms. Res Autism Spectr Disord 2012; 6(1): 277-85.
47
Zhang-James Y, Razavi AS, Hoogman M, Franke B, Faraone SV. Machine learning and MRI-based diagnostic models for ADHD: Are we there yet? J Atten Disord 2023; 27(4): 335-53.
48
Kautzky A, Vanicek T, Philippe C, et al. Machine learning classification of ADHD and HC by multimodal serotonergic data. Transl Psychiatry 2020; 10(1): 104.
49
Pereira-Sanchez V, Castellanos FX. Neuroimaging in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Curr Opin Psychiatry 2021; 34(2): 105-11.
50
Mikolas P, Vahid A, Bernardoni F, et al. Training a machine learning classifier to identify ADHD based on real-world clinical data from medical records. Sci Rep 2022; 12(1): 12934.
51
Pergamit MR, Pierret CR, Rothstein DS, Veum JR. Data watch: The national longitudinal surveys. J Econ Perspect 2001; 15(2): 239-53.
52
Cooksey E. The NLSY for new and returning users. PAA 2017 Annual Meeting 27–29 April 2027; Chicago Hilton, IL. 2017.2017.
53
Peterson JL, Zill N. Marital disruption, parent-child relationships, and behavior problems in children. J Marriage Fam 1986; 48(2): 295-307.
54
Achenbach TM, Edelbrock CS. Behavioral problems and competencies reported by parents of normal and disturbed children aged four through sixteen. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 1981; 46(1): 1-82.
55
Rutter M, Graham P. The reliability and validity of the psychiatric assessment of the child: I. Interview with the child. Br J Psychiatry 1968; 114(510): 563-79.
56
Kellam SG. Mental health and going to school: The Woodlawn program of assessment, early intervention, and evaluation 1975.
57
Rutter M, Tizard J, Whitmore K. Education, health, and behaviour 1970.
58
Craig BM, Brown DS, Reeve BB. Valuation of child behavioral problems from the perspective of US adults. Med Decis Making 2016; 36(2): 199-209.
59
Nagin D. Group-based modeling of development 2005.
60
Nguena Nguefack HL, Pagé MG, Katz J, et al. Trajectory modelling techniques useful to epidemiological research: A comparative narrative review of approaches. Clin Epidemiol 2020; 12: 1205-22.
61
Nagin DS. Group-based trajectory modeling: An overview.Handbook of Quantitative Criminology 2010.
62
Frankfurt S, Frazier P, Syed M, Jung KR. Using group-based trajectory and growth mixture modeling to identify classes of change trajectories. Couns Psychol 2016; 44(5): 622-60.
63
Niyonkuru C, Wagner AK, Ozawa H, Amin K, Goyal A, Fabio A. Group-based trajectory analysis applications for prognostic biomarker model development in severe TBI: A practical example. J Neurotrauma 2013; 30(11): 938-45.
64
Goyal A, Niyonkuru C, Carter M, Fabio A, Berger R, Wagner A. Comparative assessment of serum and CSF S100B profiles in outcome prediction. J Neurotrauma 2013; 30(11): 946-57.
65
Roeder K, Lynch KG, Nagin DS. Modeling uncertainty in latent class membership: A case study in criminology. J Am Stat Assoc 1999; 94(447): 766-76.
66
Barnett TA, Gauvin L, Craig CL, Katzmarzyk PT. Distinct trajectories of leisure time physical activity and predictors of trajectory class membership: A 22 year cohort study. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 2008; 5(1): 57.
67
Shi Q, Mendoza TR, Gunn GB, Wang XS, Rosenthal DI, Cleeland CS. Using group-based trajectory modeling to examine heterogeneity of symptom burden in patients with head and neck cancer undergoing aggressive non-surgical therapy. Qual Life Res 2013; 22(9): 2331-9.
68
Rabbitts JA, Zhou C, Groenewald CB, Durkin L, Palermo TM. Trajectories of postsurgical pain in children. Pain 2015; 156(11): 2383-9.
69
Arling G, Ofner S, Reeves MJ, et al. Care trajectories of veterans in the 12 months after hospitalization for acute ischemic stroke. Circ Cardiovasc Qual Outcomes 2015; 8(6_suppl_3)(Suppl. 3): S131-40.
70
Arrandale V, Koehoorn M, MacNab Y, Kennedy SM. How to use SAS® Proc Traj and SAS® Proc Glimmix in respiratory epidemiology 2006.
71
Kass RE, Raftery AE. Bayes Factors. J Am Stat Assoc 1995; 90(430): 773-95.
72
Kessler RC, Adler L, Barkley R, et al. The prevalence and correlates of adult ADHD in the United States: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163(4): 716-23.
73
Stibbe T, Huang J, Paucke M, Ulke C, Strauss M. Gender differences in adult ADHD: Cognitive function assessed by the test of attentional performance. PLoS One 2020; 15(10): e0240810.
74
Retz W, Ginsberg Y, Turner D, et al. Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), antisociality and delinquent behavior over the lifespan. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2021; 120: 236-48.
75
Storebø OJ, Simonsen E. The association between ADHD and antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) a review. J Atten Disord 2016; 20(10): 815-24.
76
Figueiredo T, Lima G, Erthal P, et al. Mind-wandering, depression, anxiety and ADHD: Disentangling the relationship. Psychiatry Res 2020; 285: 112798.
77
Paek SH, Abdulla AM, Cramond B. A meta-analysis of the relationship between three common psychopathologies—ADHD, anxiety, and depression—and indicators of little-c creativity. Gift Child Q 2016; 60(2): 117-33.
78
Riglin L, Leppert B, Dardani C, et al. ADHD and depression: Investigating a causal explanation. Psychol Med 2021; 51(11): 1890-7.
79
Jarrett MA, Ollendick TH. A conceptual review of the comorbidity of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and anxiety: Implications for future research and practice. Clin Psychol Rev 2008; 28(7): 1266-80.
80
Overgaard KR, Aase H, Torgersen S, Zeiner P. Co-occurrence of ADHD and anxiety in preschool children. J Atten Disord 2016; 20(7): 573-80.
81
Boschman JS, Nieuwenhuijsen K, Frings-Dresen MHW, Sluiter JK. Development of hospital nurses’ work ability over a 2 year period. Occup Med (Lond) 2015; 65(7): 542-8.
82
Tsai CJ, Chen YL, Lin HY, Gau SSF. One-year trajectory analysis for ADHD symptoms and its associated factors in community-based children and adolescents in Taiwan. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health 2017; 11(1): 28.
83
Murray AL, Hall HA, Speyer LG, et al. Developmental trajectories of ADHD symptoms in a large population-representative longitudinal study. Psychol Med 2021; 52(15): 1-7.
84
Tandon M, Tillman R, Agrawal A, Luby J. Trajectories of ADHD severity over 10 years from childhood into adulthood. Atten Defic Hyperact Disord 2016; 8(3): 121-30.
85
Leopold DR, Christopher ME, Burns GL, Becker SP, Olson RK, Willcutt EG. Attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder and sluggish cognitive tempo throughout childhood: Temporal invariance and stability from preschool through ninth grade. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2016; 57(9): 1066-74.
86
Sasser TR, Beekman CR III, Bierman KL. Preschool executive functions, single-parent status, and school quality predict diverging trajectories of classroom inattention in elementary school. Dev Psychopathol 2015; 27(3): 681-93.
87
O’Neill S, Rajendran K, Mahbubani SM, Halperin JM. Preschool predictors of ADHD symptoms and impairment during childhood and adolescence. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2017; 19(12): 95.
88
Willoughby MT, Pek J, Greenberg MT, Investigators FLP. Parent-reported Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity symptomatology in preschool-aged children: Factor structure, developmental change, and early risk factors. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2012; 40(8): 1301-12.
89
Murray AL, Booth T, Auyeung B, Eisner M, Ribeaud D, Obsuth I. Outcomes of ADHD symptoms in late adolescence: Are developmental subtypes important? J Atten Disord 2020; 24(1): 113-25.
90
Sasser TR, Kalvin CB, Bierman KL. Developmental trajectories of clinically significant ADHD symptoms from grade 3 through 12 in a high-risk sample: Predictors and outcomes. J Abnorm Psychol 2016; 125(2): 207.
91
Daaleman T P, Preisser J. A life course perspective on behavior and health.Principles And Concepts of Behavioral Medicine: A Global Handbook 2018.
92
Morgan PL, Staff J, Hillemeier MM, Farkas G, Maczuga S. Racial and ethnic disparities in ADHD diagnosis from kindergarten to eighth grade. Pediatrics 2013; 132(1): 85-93.
93
Coker TR, Elliott MN, Toomey SL, et al. Racial and ethnic disparities in ADHD diagnosis and treatment. Pediatrics 2016; 138(3): e20160407.
94
Tseng PT, Yen CF, Chen YW, et al. Maternal breastfeeding and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children: A meta-analysis. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2019; 28(1): 19-30.
95
Soled D, Keim SA, Rapoport E, Rosen L, Adesman A. Breastfeeding is associated with a reduced risk of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder among preschool children. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2021; 42(1): 9-15.
96
Huang L, Wang Y, Zhang L, et al. Maternal smoking and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in offspring: A meta-analysis. Pediatrics 2018; 141(1): e20172465.
97
Sourander A, Sucksdorff M, Chudal R, et al. Prenatal cotinine levels and ADHD among offspring. Pediatrics 2019; 143(3): e20183144.
98
Breaux RP, Harvey EA, Lugo-Candelas CI. The role of parent psychopathology in the development of preschool children with behavior problems. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 2014; 43(5): 777-90.
99
Feil EG, Small JW, Seeley JR, et al. Early intervention for preschoolers at risk for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Preschool first step to success. Behav Disord 2016; 41(2): 95-106.
100
Pinkert A T. Early preventive interventions for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A systematic literature review 2020.
101
Daniel GR, Wang C, Berthelsen D. Early school-based parent involvement, children’s self-regulated learning and academic achievement: An Australian longitudinal study. Early Child Res Q 2016; 36: 168-77.
102
Mazzone L, Postorino V, Reale L, et al. Self-esteem evaluation in children and adolescents suffering from ADHD. Clin Pract Epidemiol Ment Health 2013; 9(1): 96-102.
103
Becker K, Banaschewski T, Brandeis D, et al. Individualised stepwise adaptive treatment for 3–6-year-old preschool children impaired by attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ESCApreschool): Study protocol of an adaptive intervention study including two randomised controlled trials within the consortium ESCAlife. Trials 2020; 21(1): 56.